J. MICHAEL SEABRIGHT, Chief District Judge.
The United States seeks an order in limine excluding Dr. Elizabeth Loftus ("Loftus") from testifying as an expert witness regarding the corruption of memory and related topics, pursuant to Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 509 U.S. 579 (1993) ("Daubert I"). ECF No. 394. On March 6, 2019, the court held an evidentiary hearing, with Loftus as the sole witness. For the reasons set forth below, the motion is GRANTED.
The First Superseding Indictment ("FSI") charges Defendant Gordon Shiraishi ("Shiraishi") with several offenses alleging that he provided material false statements concerning the time of day on June 22, 2013 that former Honolulu Police Department ("HPD") Chief Louis Kealoha ("Kealoha") telephoned him and reported that his home mailbox had been stolen. In particular, Shiraishi is alleged to have repeated the same general false statement — that he received a phone call from Kealoha around 9 a.m. on June 22, 2013 and then called HPD Lieutenant Derek Hahn and directed Hahn to send a technician to the Kealoha residence — on four separate occasions: 1) on April 30, 2015 before the Honolulu Ethics Commission ("Ethics Commission"), FSI at ¶ 37uu, ECF No. 164;
During the April 30, 2015 Ethics Commission interview, Shiraishi stated that his phone call with Kealoha "was in the morning time, I think, about around 9 or after; 9 a.m[.], sorry." ECF No. 445-2 at Page ID # 3827 (79:22-23).
The November 16, 2015 FBI interview is memorialized in an FBI 302, which states in part that "[o]n June 22, 2013, SHIRAISHI learned the mailbox was stolen. SHIRAISHI was at home, as it was a Saturday. [Kealoha] called SHIRAISHI about 9:00AM to inform him about the mailbox. . . ." ECF No. 445-3 at PageID # 3832.
On January 6, 2016, the day prior to his scheduled grand jury appearance, FBI special agent Nicole Vallieres ("Vallieres") interviewed Shiraishi. Although no FBI 302 appears to have been generated, the government submitted Vallieres' handwritten notes from that interview. ECF No. 477-1. At the very beginning of the notes (that is, on the top of the first page), Vallieres wrote:
Id. at PageID # 4162. On the third page of her notes (that is, later during the interview), she wrote:
Id. at PageID # 4164. The parties agree that the Niall Silva "form" shown to Shiraishi on January 6, 2016 refers to a July 1, 2013 "Honolulu Police Department Follow Up Report" completed by former HPD Corporal Niall Silva ("Silva follow-up report"). In that report, Silva states he recovered video footage from the Kealoha residence "on 06/22/13 at about 0859 hours." ECF No. 445-4. The parties also agree that the Silva follow-up report is false. Specifically, the parties agree that Silva lied on this report, and in fact he did not recover video footage from the Kealoha residence "at about 0859 hours" on June 22, 2013.
On January 7, 2016, Shiraishi testified before Grand Jury 15-1, and was again shown Silva's follow-up report. ECF No. 445-10. But before being shown the Silva follow-up report, Shiraishi testified as follows:
Id. at PageID # 4052 (11:13-22).
Later, after Shiraishi was shown the Silva follow-up report, Shiraishi testified in the grand jury as follows:
Id. at PageID # 4061-63 (19:2-8; 19:13-15; 19:20-24; 20:4-7; 20:15-21:2).
Federal Rule of Evidence 702 provides:
The court has the responsibility of acting as a gatekeeper to prevent unreliable expert testimony from reaching the jury. Daubert I, 509 U.S. at 589. In carrying out this responsibility, the court has discretion and flexibility in determining what evidence is relevant, reliable, and helpful to the trier of fact. Cabrera v. Cordis Corp., 134 F.3d 1418, 1420 (9th Cir. 1998); United States v. Cordoba, 104 F.3d 225, 228 (9th Cir. 1997) ("District courts must strike the appropriate balance between admitting reliable, helpful expert testimony and excluding misleading or confusing testimony to achieve the flexible approach outlined in Daubert.") (quoting United States v. Rincon, 28 F.3d 921, 926 (9th Cir. 1994)).
The Ninth Circuit has articulated a two-prong analysis for admissibility. First, the proffered testimony must be reliable, i.e., the expert's testimony reflects scientific knowledge, the findings are derived by the scientific method, and the work product amounts to "good science." Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., 43 F.3d 1311, 1315 (9th Cir. 1995) ("Daubert II") (citation and quotation signals omitted). Second, the testimony must meet the "fit" requirement, i.e., "it logically advances a material aspect of the proposing party's case." Id.
For the reliability inquiry, the focus is on the expert's "principles and methodology, not on the conclusions that they generate." Daubert I, 509 U.S. at 595. "Scientific evidence is deemed reliable if the principles and methodology used by an expert are grounded in the methods of science." Clausen v. M/V New Carissa, 339 F.3d 1049, 1056 (9th Cir. 2003); see also Wendell v. GlaxoSmithKline LLC, 858 F.3d 1227, 1232 (9th Cir. 2017). Accordingly, the expert's methods must be adequately explained. United States v. Hermanek, 289 F.3d 1076, 1094 (9th Cir. 2002); see also Daubert II, 43 F.3d at 1319 (holding that the expert must "explain the methodology . . . followed to reach [his or her] conclusions"); Rincon, 28 F.3d at 924 (explaining that the methods used by the expert must be described "in sufficient detail" such that the district court can determine if they are reliable). "For scientific opinion, the court must assess the reasoning or methodology, using as appropriate such criteria as testability, publication in peer reviewed literature, and general acceptance, but the inquiry is a flexible one. Shaky but admissible evidence is to be attacked by cross examination, contrary evidence, and attention to the burden of proof, not exclusion." Primiano v. Cook, 598 F.3d 558, 564 (9th Cir. 2010) (footnotes omitted); see also Murray v. S. Route Mar. SA, 870 F.3d 915, 925 (9th Cir. 2017).
For the "fit" inquiry, the focus is "primarily" on "relevance." Daubert I, 509 U.S. at 591. "`Expert testimony which does not relate to any issue in the case is not relevant and, ergo, non-helpful.'" Id. (quoting 3 Weinstein & Berger ¶ 702[02], p. 702-18). But this inquiry is not merely a reiteration of the relevancy inquiry — "[e]xpert evidence can be both powerful and quite misleading because of the difficulty in evaluating it. Because of this risk, the judge in weighing possible prejudice against probative force under Rule 403 of the present rules exercises more control over experts than over lay witnesses." Id. at 595 (citation and quotation signals omitted). "Federal judges must therefore exclude proffered scientific evidence under Rules 702 and 403 unless they are convinced that [the evidence] speaks clearly and directly to an issue in dispute in the case, and that it will not mislead the jury." Daubert II, 43 F.3d at 1321 n.17. And to be admissible "the subject matter at issue must be beyond the common knowledge of the average layman." United States v. Finley, 301 F.3d 1000, 1007 (9th Cir. 2002); United States v. Hanna, 293 F.3d 1080, 1086 (9th Cir. 2002).
Applying these standards, the court finds that Loftus' proffered testimony meets the "reliability" requirement, but fails the "fit" requirement because the testimony does not "logically advance[] a material aspect of" Shiraishi's case. Daubert II, 43 F.3d at 1315.
Loftus' testimony meets the "reliability" requirement. Loftus, a psychology professor at the University of California Irvine, is an expert in the study of human memory and a leading authority on how memory can be corrupted. See Dennis v. Sec'y, Pa. Dep't of Corr., 834 F.3d 263, 325 (3d Cir. 2016) ("Elizabeth Loftus, a pioneering researcher in the field of human memory and cognition, has thoroughly documented the effects of received information on memory accuracy."). While initially reluctant to allow testimony about the malleability of memory, many courts now admit testimony similar to that proferred by Loftus. See United States v. Rodriguez-Felix, 450 F.3d 1117, 1123-24 (10th Cir. 2006) ("[M]odern research also recognizes that an eyewitness's identification may be subject to significant witness error or manipulation. . . . Initially, courts were skeptical of this research, and almost uniformly rejected it. Many courts now, however, are proving receptive to admitting testimony based on this research.") (citing an article authored by Loftus — Elizabeth F. Loftus & James M. Doyle, Eyewitness Testimony § 1-1 (3d ed. 1997 & Supp. 2005)); see also United States v. Valencia-Cortez, 2018 WL 6445875, at *2 (9th Cir. Dec. 10, 2018) (recognizing the "particularly unreliable nature of eyewitness identification evidence" and that it is "within a court's sound discretion to provide both safeguards [(expert witness testimony and a comprehensive eyewitness jury instruction)] if the facts and circumstances of the case so require").
During the Daubert hearing, Loftus explained the "misinformation effect," where memories can be altered after receiving false post-event information. Loftus also testified about "confidence inflation" and how a person's confidence in a memory is malleable and exposure to new information may inflate a person's confidence in a memory. Thus, exposure to misinformation can impact both the recollection of a memory and the level of confidence in that memory recall.
In explaining these theories, Loftus relied on peer-reviewed articles that discussed a number of studies in these areas. See, e.g., ECF No. 445-7 (peer-reviewed article reviewing studies about the malleability of memory); ECF No. 445-8 (peer-reviewed article reviewing studies about contamination of eyewitness self-reports). Loftus has conducted a number of these studies herself and is well-versed in the work of others in the field. Thus, the court finds that Loftus' theories on the malleability of human memory are reliable in that they are, among other reasons, "generally accepted in the scientific community," and have "been subjected to peer review and publication." Daubert II, 43 F.3d at 1316; see, e.g., Johnson v. Chappelle, 2012 WL 1094345, at *3 (N.D. Cal. Mar. 30, 2012) (describing Loftus' expert testimony, which was admitted by the trial court, about "factors affecting human perception, memory, and identification"); cf. Rodriguez-Felix, 450 F.3d at 1125-26 ("The description of Dr. Clark's research [on factors affecting eyewitness identification] is wholly inadequate — it fails to indicate whether it has been subjected to peer review, whether it has been published, and whether it has been accepted by other psychologists in the field.").
Next, the court turns to Daubert's "fit" requirement. Shiraishi seeks Loftus' testimony to explain to the jury why Shiraishi may have claimed, during four separate interviews, that he spoke to Kealoha at approximately 9:00 a.m. on June 22, 2013 (when, in fact, that conversation was in the afternoon and evening). Loftus testified that providing Shiraishi with the false Silva follow-up report could affect his confidence in his memory as to the time he received the June 22, 2013 Kealoha phone call. For example, she explained that exposure to the false Silva follow-up report (during the FBI interview on January 6, 2016 and during the grand jury appearance on January 7, 2016) could impact Shiraishi's recollection and his confidence in that recollection, and that "one likely effect" of exposure to this misinformation would be to make Shiraishi more confident that the Kealoha phone called occurred prior to 9:00 a.m. Finally, Loftus explained that individuals gain confidence in a memory after engaging in repeated recollections of that particular memory (even absent being provided with any false or misleading information).
Shiraishi thus argues that Loftus' explanation of the "misinformation effect" and "confidence inflation" may be helpful to the jury because his recollection of the time of the June 22, 2013 Kealoha phone call may have been altered in some way when shown the false Silva follow-up report. But the first three statements (one to the Ethics Commission and two to the FBI) could not reflect a corrupted memory — each was made prior to Shiraishi being shown the Silva follow-up report. That leaves, as Loftus testified, that exposure to the Silva follow-up report may have "solidified" Shiraishi's earlier estimate of the time of the Kealoha phone call. But this theory does not "fit" the evidence before the court — Shiraishi expressed his memory as to the time of the Kealoha phone call, and the confidence in that memory, in similar terms before and after reviewing the Silva follow-up report.
During his Ethics Commission interview, Shiraishi stated that the Kealoha phone call "was in the morning time, I think, about around 9 or after; 9 a.m[.], sorry." ECF No. 445-2 at PageID # 3827 (79:22-23). When then asked if Kealoha called him at about 9:00 a.m., he responded "[a]round there or maybe after. Wasn't like early in the morning." Id. at PageID # 3828 (80:2). Next, on November 16, 2015, Shiraishi reportedly told the FBI that the Kealoha phone call took place "at about 9:00 a.m." ECF No. 445-3 at PageID # 3832. And then, on January 6, 2016, Shiraishi told Vallieres that he received the Kealoha call on Saturday morning while watering his lawn at "9:00 ish." ECF No. 477-1 at PageID # 4162. Only after these three statements — all placing the time of the Kealoha call "about around" 9:00 a.m., or "at about 9:00 a.m." or "9:00 ish" was Shiraishi shown the Silva follow-up report. And, most importantly, his testimony was strikingly similar even after shown the Silva follow-up report — in front of the grand jury, he testified that the Kealoha call was "in the morning time, 9:00 — after 9:00 or before 9:00 I can't tell you the exact time. But it was in the morning, and I was watering my yard." ECF No. 445-10 at PageID # 4053 (11:19-21). This approximation of the time is strikingly similar to "about around 9" or "at about 9:00 a.m." or "9:00 ish." In short, there was simply no "misinformation effect" or "confidence inflation." Instead, in each instance when asked about the time of the Kealoha phone call, whether before or after shown the Silva follow-up report, Shiraishi was clear that he was providing an estimated time — around 9:00 a.m.
Further, Silva's follow-up report states that he picked up the video surveillance footage at 8:59 a.m., which necessarily would have been after (not before) Shiraishi initiated the investigation based on the Kealoha phone call. But Shiraishi never adopted Silva's version of events. Both before and after Shiraishi was presented with Silva's follow-up report, Shiraishi consistently testified that he received the phone call around 9 a.m. in the morning.
Finally, Shiraishi also appears to argue that Loftus' testimony
In sum, the court does not question Loftus' expertise or qualifications. And the court certainly agrees that, in the right case, her testimony may aid a jury in understanding "misinformation effect" and "confidence inflation." But her testimony is simply not a fit for this case where the record does not show that Shiraishi's memory changed during the relevant time period. Thus, the court must exercise its gate-keeping role under Daubert and exclude Loftus' expert testimony.
For the foregoing reasons, the United States' Motion in Limine is GRANTED.
IT IS SO ORDERED.